user since
Tue Oct 7 2003 at 23:00:40 (21.1 years ago )
last seen
Sun Nov 13 2005 at 02:49:17 (19 years ago )
level / experience
0 (Initiate) / 0
Send private message to effects

Rejection really hurts finds brain study 
 
  
19:00 09 October 03 
  
NewScientist.com news service 
  
Lonely hearts have spent millennia trying to capture the pain of rejection in 
painting, poetry and song. Now neuroscientists have seen it flickering in some 
remarkable brain images from college students suffering a social snub.

The brain scans reveal that two of the same brain regions that are activated 
by physical pain are also activated by social exclusion.

"This doesn't mean a broken arm hurts exactly the same way that a broken heart 
does," says Matthew Lieberman of the University of California, Los Angeles, 
who led the research. "But it shows that the human brain sounds the same alarm 
system for emotional and physical distress." 

Eventually, by targeting drugs to these regions, he says it may be possible to 
develop powerful new medications for extreme cases of social anxiety or 
depression.

"This is evidence that humans don't build complex emotions out of thin air," 
says Jaak Panksepp, of Northwestern University in Evanston, Illinois, who has 
studied how the same regions are involved in social interactions in many 
mammals. "These emotions are built on basic tools that evolution gave us a 
long time ago. " 


Emotional alarm system 


Lieberman describes the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) as an emotional alarm 
system that draws the brain's attention to distressing or unexpected changes 
in the environment. The region lights up in response to pain, for instance, 
but also when a mother hear an infant's cry. But no one had tested whether 
social rejection also activated this area in humans.

So the researchers used functional magnetic resonance imaging to view the 
brains of nine female and four male college students who volunteered to play a 
computer game. In the game, they caught and threw a ball with two other 
players. Each participant was told they were interacting with other students, 
but in fact the other players were controlled by the computer.


In the first part of the experiment, the participant was told technical 
problems prevented them from playing, so they could only watch. In the second 
half, they were able to catch and throw the ball to the player of their 
choosing. But after they received the ball seven times, the computer stopped 
throwing the ball to them for the remaining 40 or so tosses. 


Afterwards, each student was asked about their level of distress at being 
given the cold shoulder by the other players.


Thirst and hunger 
 
 
  
In both parts of the experiment, the ACC lit up and was more active in 
students who reported greater distress. During the second experiment, another 
region called the right ventral prefrontal cortex, which animal studies have 
linked to reducing suffering from pain, was also activated. 

Lieberman speculates that this region also helps cope with the "pain" of 
social exclusion, but may only be activated when a cause can be identified, 
which was not the case in the first experiment.

The fact that disruption of even a trivial virtual relationship can activate 
the ACC shows how hard wired the response is. "The need for social 
connectiveness isn't just something self-help authors cooked up," says 
Lieberman. "It's a basic need programmed into a primitive part of our brains 
like thirst and pain and hunger."

Science (vol 302, p 290)